Fri . 19 Jul 2019
TR | RU | UK | KK | BE |

Mobbing (animal behavior)


Mobbing in animals is an antipredator adaptation in which individuals of prey species mob a predator by cooperatively attacking or harassing it, usually to protect their offspring A simple definition of mobbing is an assemblage of individuals around a potentially dangerous predator This is most frequently seen in birds, though it is also known to occur in many other animals such as the meerkat While mobbing has evolved independently in many species, it only tends to be present in those whose young are frequently preyed upon This behavior may complement cryptic adaptations in the offspring themselves, such as camouflage and hiding Mobbing calls may be used to summon nearby individuals to cooperate in the attack

Konrad Lorenz, in his book On Aggression 1966, attributed mobbing among birds and animals to instincts rooted in the Darwinian struggle to survive In his view, humans are subject to similar innate impulses but capable of bringing them under rational control see mobbing

Contents

  • 1 In birds
  • 2 In other animals
  • 3 Mobbing calls
  • 4 Evolution
  • 5 References
  • 6 External links

In birds

A great kiskadee right mobs a hawk A great black-backed gull, Larus marinus, mobbing a far larger Euroasian sea eagle, Haliateus albicilla Crows mobbing a perched bald eagle A crow engaging in dive-bombing behaviour upon a bald eagle

Birds that breed in colonies such as gulls are widely seen to attack intruders, including encroaching humans In North America, the birds that most frequently engage in mobbing include mockingbirds, crows and jays, chickadees, terns, and blackbirds Behavior includes flying about the intruder, dive bombing, loud squawking and defecating on the predator Mobbing can also be used to obtain food, by driving larger birds and mammals away from a food source, or by harassing a bird with food One bird might distract while others quickly steal food Scavenging birds such as gulls frequently use this technique to steal food from humans nearby A flock of birds might drive a powerful animal away from food Costs of mobbing behavior include the risk of engaging with predators, as well as energy expended in the process The black-headed gull is a species which aggressively engages intruding predators, such as carrion crows Classic experiments on this species by Hans Kruuk involved placing hen eggs at intervals from a nesting colony, and recording the percentage of successful predation events as well as the probability of the crow being subjected to mobbing The results showed decreasing mobbing with increased distance from the nest, which was correlated with increased predation success Mobbing may function by reducing the predator's ability to locate nests as a distraction since predators cannot focus on locating eggs while they are under attack

Besides the ability to drive the predator away, mobbing also draws attention to the predator, making stealth attacks impossible Mobbing plays a critical role in the identification of predators and inter-generational learning about predator identification Reintroduction of species is often unsuccessful, because the established population lacks this cultural knowledge of how to identify local predators Scientists are exploring ways to train populations to identify and respond to predators before releasing them into the wild

Adaptationist hypotheses regarding why an organism should engage in such risky behavior have been suggested by Eberhard Curio, including advertising their physical fitness and hence uncatchability much like stotting behavior in gazelles, distracting predators from finding their offspring, warning their offspring, luring the predator away, allowing offspring to learn to recognize the predator species, directly injuring the predator or attracting a predator of the predator itself The much lower frequency of attacks between nesting seasons suggests such behavior may have evolved due to its benefit for the mobber's young Niko Tinbergen argued that the mobbing was a source of confusion to gull chick predators, distracting them from searching for prey Indeed, an intruding carrion crow can only avoid incoming attacks by facing its attackers, which prevents it from locating its target

Besides experimental research, the comparative method can also be employed to investigate hypotheses such as those given by Curio above For example, closely related species such as the kittiwake do not show mobbing behavior The kittiwake's cliff nests are almost completely inaccessible to possible predators due to gusty winds and sheer cliffs, meaning its young are not at risk to predation like the black-headed gull This is an example of an evolutionary pattern known as divergent evolution

Mobbing is thought to carry risks to roosting predators, including potential harm from the mobbing birds, or attracting larger, more dangerous predators Birds at risk of mobbing such as owls have cryptic plumage and hidden roosts which reduces this danger

In other animals

The occurrence of mobbing behavior across widely different taxa, including California ground squirrels, is evidence of convergent evolution

Another way the comparative method can be used here is by comparing gulls with distantly related organisms This approach relies on the existence of convergent evolution, where distantly related organisms evolve the same trait due to similar selection pressures As mentioned, many bird species such as the swallows also mob predators, however more distantly related groups including mammals have been known to engage in this behavior One example is the California ground squirrel, which distracts predators such as the rattlesnake and gopher snake from locating their nest burrows by kicking sand into their face, which disrupts the snake's sensory organs; for crotaline snakes, this includes the heat-detecting organs in the loreal pits This social species also uses alarm calls

Some fish engage in mobbing; for example, bluegills sometimes attack snapping turtles Bluegills, which form large nesting colonies, were seen to attack both released and naturally occurring turtles, which may advertise their presence, drive the predator from the area, or aid in the transmission of predator recognition Similarly, humpback whales are known to mob killer whales when the latter are attacking other species, including other cetacean species, seals, sea lions, and fish

Mobbing calls

The great tit P major, a passerine bird, employs both mobbing behavior and alarm calls

Mobbing calls are signals made by the mobbing species while harassing a predator These differ from alarm calls, which allow con-specifics to escape from the predator The great tit, a European songbird, uses such a signal to call on nearby birds to harass a perched bird of prey, such as an owl This call occurs in the 45kHz range, and carries over long distances However, when their prey are in flight, they employ an alarm signal in the 7–8 kHz range This call is less effective at traveling great distances, but is much more difficult for both owls and hawks to hear and detect the direction from which the call came In the case of the alarm call, it could be disadvantageous to the sender if the predator picks up on the signal, hence selection has favored those birds able to hear and employ calls in this higher frequency range

Mobbing calls may also be part of an animal's arsenal in harassing the predator Studies of Phainopepla mobbing calls indicate it may serve to enhance the swooping attack on the predators, including scrub jays In this species, the mobbing call is smoothly upsweeping, and is made when swooping down in an arc beside the predator This call was also heard during agonistic behavior interactions with conspecifics, and may serve additionally or alternatively as an alarm call to their mate

Evolution

African buffalo herd confronting a lion

The evolution of mobbing behavior can be explained using evolutionarily stable strategies, which are in turn based on game theory

Mobbing involves risks costs to the individual and benefits payoffs to the individual and others The individuals themselves are often genetically related, and mobbing is increasingly studied with the gene-centered view of evolution by considering inclusive fitness the carrying on of one's genes through one's family members, rather than merely benefit to the individual

By cooperating to successfully drive away predators, all individuals involved increase their chances of survival and reproduction An individual stands little chance against a larger predator, but when a large group is involved, the risk to each group member is reduced or diluted This so-called dilution effect proposed by W D Hamilton is another way of explaining the benefits of cooperation by selfish individuals Lanchester's laws also provide an insight into the advantages of attacking in a large group rather than individually

Another interpretation involves the use of signalling theory, and possibly the handicap principle Here the idea is that a mobbing bird, by apparently putting itself at risk, displays its status and health so as to be preferred by potential partners

References

  1. ^ a b Dominey, Wallace J 1983 "Mobbing in Colonially Nesting Fishes, Especially the Bluegill, Lepomis macrochirus" Copeia 1983 4: 1086–1088 doi:102307/1445113 JSTOR 1445113 
  2. ^ Kenneth Westheus Mobbing uwaterlooca
  3. ^ a b c Alcock, John 1998 Animal Behavior: An Evolutionary Approach 6th ed Sunderland: Sinauer Associates ISBN 0-87893-009-4 
  4. ^ Kruuk, H 1964 Predators and anti-predator behaviour of the black-headed gull Larus ridibundus Behaviour Supplements 11 Leiden: EJ Brill OCLC 1502972 
  5. ^ Griffin, Andrea S; Daniel T Blumstein; Christopher S Evans October 2000 "Training Captive-Bred or Translocated Animals to Avoid Predators" Conservation Biology 14 5: 1317–1326 doi:101046/j1523-1739200099326x 
  6. ^ Curio, E 1978 "The adaptive significance of avian mobbing I Teleonomic hypotheses and predictions" Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 48: 175–183 
  7. ^ Curio, E; U Ernst; W Vieth 1978 "Cultural Transmission of Enemy Recognition: One Function of Mobbing" Science 202 4370: 899–901 doi:101126/science2024370899 PMID 17752463 
  8. ^ Tinbergen, Niko The herring gull's world: a study of the social behavior of birds New York: Lyons and Bulford ISBN 1-55821-049-0 
  9. ^ Cullen, E 1957 "Adaptations in the kittiwake to cliff nesting" Ibis 99 2: 275–302 doi:101111/j1474-919X1957tb01950x 
  10. ^ Hendrichsen, Ditte K; Christiansen, Peter; Nielsen, Elsemarie K; Dabelsteen, Torben; Sunde, Peter 2006 "Exposure affects the risk of an owl being mobbed – experimental evidence" Journal of Avian Biology 37 1: 13–18 doi:101111/j20050908-885703658x 
  11. ^ Coss, Richard G; Biardi, J E 1997 "Individual Variation in the Antisnake Behavior of California Ground Squirrels Spermophilus beecheyi" Journal of Mammalogy 78 2: 294–310 doi:102307/1382883 JSTOR 1382883 
  12. ^ Pitman, Robert; et al 2016-07-20 "Humpback whales interfering when mammal-eating killer whales attack other species: Mobbing behavior and interspecific altruism" Marine Mammal Science doi:101111/mms12343 Retrieved 2016-08-02 – via Wiley Online Library  CS1 maint: Explicit use of et al link
  13. ^ Brown, C H 1982 "Ventriloquial and locatable vocalizations in birds" Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologies 59: 338–350 doi:101111/j1439-03101982tb00346x 
  14. ^ Leger, Daniel W; Laura F Carroll 1981 "Mobbing Calls of the Phainopepla" PDF The Condor 83 4: 377–380 doi:102307/1367509 JSTOR 1367509 Retrieved 2007-06-12 
  15. ^ Parker, Geoffrey A, Manfred Milinski 1997 "Cooperation under Predation Risk: A Data-Based ESS Analysis" Proceedings: Biological Sciences 264 1385: 1239–1247 doi:101098/rspb19970171  CS1 maint: Multiple names: authors list link
  16. ^ Kelly, Kevin 1994 Out of control: the new biology of machines, social systems and the economic world Boston: Addison-Wesley ISBN 0-201-48340-8 
  17. ^ Hamilton, W D 1971 "Geometry for the selfish herd" PDF Journal of Theoretical Biology 31 2: 295–311 doi:101016/0022-51937190189-5 PMID 5104951 
  18. ^ Arnold, K E 2000 "Group Mobbing Behaviour and Nest Defence in a Cooperatively Breeding Australian Bird" PDF Ethology 106 5: 385–393 doi:101046/j1439-0310200000545x 

External links

  • Interspecific reciprocity explains mobbing behaviour of the breeding chaffinches, Fringilla coelebs Paper by Indrikis Krams and Tatjana Krama PDF
  • Nature Photography – Using mobbing behavior in photography
  • Birds mob Puff Adder – paper in ejournal Ornithological Observations


Mobbing (animal behavior) Information about

Mobbing (animal behavior)


  • user icon

    Mobbing (animal behavior) beatiful post thanks!

    29.10.2014


Mobbing (animal behavior)
Mobbing (animal behavior)
Mobbing (animal behavior) viewing the topic.
Mobbing (animal behavior) what, Mobbing (animal behavior) who, Mobbing (animal behavior) explanation

There are excerpts from wikipedia on this article and video

Random Posts

B♭ (musical note)

B♭ (musical note)

B♭ B-flat; also called si bémol is the eleventh step of the Western chromatic scale starting from C ...
Fourth dimension in art

Fourth dimension in art

New possibilities opened up by the concept of four-dimensional space and difficulties involved in tr...
Holt Renfrew

Holt Renfrew

Holt, Renfrew & Co, Limited, commonly known as Holt Renfrew or Holt's,1 is a chain of high-end C...
Later Silla

Later Silla

Later Silla 668–935, Hangul: 후신라; Hanja: 後新羅; RR: Hushila, Korean pronunciation: ...